Breaking the reading code: Letter knowledge when children break the reading code the first year in school

dc.contributorHáskólinn í Reykjavíken_US
dc.contributorReykjavik Universityen_US
dc.contributor.authorSigmundsson, Hermundur
dc.contributor.authorHaga, Monika
dc.contributor.authorOfteland, Greta Storm
dc.contributor.authorSolstad, Trygve
dc.contributor.departmentÍþróttafræðideild (HR)en_US
dc.contributor.departmentDepartment of Sport Science (RU)en_US
dc.contributor.schoolSamfélagssvið (HR)en_US
dc.contributor.schoolSchool of Social Sciences (RU)en_US
dc.date.accessioned2021-02-05T13:31:48Z
dc.date.available2021-02-05T13:31:48Z
dc.date.issued2020-04
dc.descriptionPublisther´s verion (útgefin grein)en_US
dc.description.abstractThe aim of this study was to examine when children learn to read and how learning to read depends on a foundation of alphabetic knowledge. 356 children aged 5–6 years completed assessments of letter-sound knowledge, i.e. the names and sounds of uppercase and lowercase letters of the Norwegian alphabet. Each child was tested at the start, the middle and the end of the school year. The time that each child broke the reading code was also recorded. The results indicated that 11% of the children knew how to read before starting school and 27% of the children did not learn to read by the end of the first year. The remaining children typically knew 21 uppercase letter sounds before they were first able to read, and only a few (<5%) knew less than 11 uppercase letter sounds when they broke the reading code. The average of all four letter-scores at the time they broke the reading code was 19 ± 5 letters (mean ± standard deviation). Although letter sound knowledge was associated with the ability to read, it was not sufficient for breaking the reading code. 40% of children who knew 23 letter sounds or more, enough to read more than 80% of the most common Norwegian words, and 15% of children who knew all 29 letter sounds still could not read. Based on these data, it seems reasonable to advocate learning letter-sound correspondences early in the first year of school to form the best possible basis for breaking the reading code.en_US
dc.description.versionPeer revieweden_US
dc.format.extent100756en_US
dc.identifier.citationSigmundsson, H., Haga, M., Ofteland, G. S., & Solstad, T. (2020). Breaking the reading code: Letter knowledge when children break the reading code the first year in school. New Ideas in Psychology, 57, 100756. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2019.100756en_US
dc.identifier.doi10.1016/j.newideapsych.2019.100756
dc.identifier.issn0732-118X
dc.identifier.issn1873-3522 (eISSN)
dc.identifier.journalNew Ideas in Psychologyen_US
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/20.500.11815/2452
dc.language.isoenen_US
dc.publisherElsevier BVen_US
dc.relation.ispartofseriesNew Ideas in Psychology;57
dc.rightsinfo:eu-repo/semantics/openAccessen_US
dc.subjectPsychology (miscellaneous)en_US
dc.subjectGeneral Psychologyen_US
dc.subjectChildrenen_US
dc.subjectGender differencesen_US
dc.subjectLetter sound knowledgeen_US
dc.subjectLongitudinal studiesen_US
dc.subjectSálfræðien_US
dc.subjectBörnen_US
dc.subjectKynjamunuren_US
dc.subjectLæsien_US
dc.subjectRittáknen_US
dc.subjectHljóðen_US
dc.titleBreaking the reading code: Letter knowledge when children break the reading code the first year in schoolen_US
dc.typeinfo:eu-repo/semantics/articleen_US
dcterms.licenseThis article is available under the Creative Commons CC-BY-NC-ND license and permits non-commercial use of the work as published, without adaptation or alteration provided the work is fully attributed. For commercial reuse, permission must be requested below.en_US

Skrár

Original bundle

Niðurstöður 1 - 1 af 1
Hleð...
Thumbnail Image
Nafn:
1-s2.0-S0732118X19301539-main.pdf
Stærð:
707.09 KB
Snið:
Adobe Portable Document Format
Description:
Publisher's version

Undirflokkur